The Open Psychology Journal




ISSN: 1874-3501 ― Volume 12, 2019

Editorial: Causal Learning Beyond Causal Judgment: An Overview


The Open Psychology Journal, 2010, 3: 88-90

Jose C. Perales, David R. Shanks

Faculty of Psychology, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Granada, Granada Spain.

Electronic publication date 13/7/2010
[DOI: 10.2174/1874350101003010088]

CrossRef Citations:
0


Article Metrics:

Geographical View
Total Statistics:

Abstract HTML Views: 326
PDF Downloads: 140
Total Views/Downloads: 580

Unique Statistics:

Abstract HTML Views: 202
PDF Downloads: 110
Total Views/Downloads: 403


Abstract:

Most research articles studying how people learn to detect causal relationships in their environments commence with some sort of example to illustrate the relevance of causality in our daily lives. These examples allude to routine problems faced by doctors, economists, social psychologists, and others and emphasize the importance of deepening our understanding of causal reasoning. But despite these frequent applied examples, it is somewhat surprising that research on causal learning has only had a modest impact in applied disciplines. After three decades or so of intense study, it is probably time to wonder why this is the case. Plainly, we do not want causal learning to become a super-specialized topic, perfectly constructed but unable to generate useful knowledge of wider relevance. In our view, cross-boundary work to fulfill this ambition is being undertaken, but to make a full impact it requires a reformulation of the implicit paradigm for causal learning research. The core of this paradigm is simple and can be summarized in two principles: first, causal knowledge can be assessed by means of verbal or numerical judgments of causal strength, and second, a single mental algorithm is sufficient to account for how environmental conditions (including covariation between cues and outcomes, time delays, and statistical interactions) map onto judgments. This research program has produced an impressive corpus of data (see [1] for a recent review), but also a current feeling that wider progress and impact is not being achieved. This special issue is a joint attempt to present a vision of how research on causal learning might develop in the future, and to push that process forward. With regard to the first principle, it is important to acknowledge that judgments are not the only way to assess causal knowledge. Judgments reflect causal beliefs, and causal beliefs are probably the basis for other responses, such as decisions or interventions. But it is not possible to predict decisions or interventions on the basis of judgments alone. It would be naive to think that causal beliefs reflected in simple causal judgments are the sole input to decision-making and intervention processes. Much effort is necessary to ascertain how causal knowledge is employed in all of these competencies, so we can build bridges between what we have discovered in recent decades and other aspects of behavior. With regard to the second principle, we argue that a reconsideration of how theory needs to develop in the future is also necessary. Most researchers now accept that people interpret the world as causal, and build mental representations of their environment in which events are causally related. Still, these causal models must be constructed from some sort of evidence, and that evidence is provided by basic coding mechanisms capturing regularities in the environment. In other words, causal learning not only serves to detect and code statistical regularities, but also to uncover the hidden causal structure that generates those regularities. For example, the correlation between smoking and lung cancer has been known for a long time. However, some scientists and tobacco manufacturers denied the existence of a causal link between the two variables, because they believed that some other causal factor was responsible for the co-occurrence (for example, populations from certain social origins could be more likely both to smoke and suffer cancer). Obviously, if smoking were not a direct cause of cancer, it would be useless to recommend people to quit smoking. In theoretical terms, we need some basic coding mechanism(s) to capture statistical regularities, and some other mechanism(s) to infer causal structures from them. Most psychologists and neuroscientists would accept that the brain is a hugely sophisticated form of connectionist net, capable of building quite reliable models of the regularities in our experiences and interactions with the world (see Moris, Cobos, & Luque's paper in this volume). Miller's comparator model [2], and Allan's [3] recent work, emphasize the idea that basic coding processes, either associative or episodic, can generate representations of the world in which, with sufficient attentional resources, most relevant events, their conjunctions, their relations of time and order, and their statistical dependencies, are conserved. Additionally, a number of algorithms have been postulated in artificial intelligence that are capable of using the sort of output generated by these basic coding mechanisms to build structural causal representations [4, 5]. The limits of bounded rationality and actual research indicate, however, that the use of such algorithms requires the management of quantities of information that are beyond human processing limits. So, causal induction is also a learning problem: certain second-order cues (abstract features of interrelations among cues) can indicate what is a cause and what is not. We, and most of the authors in this volume, support this cues-to-causality approach (see the papers by Lagnado & Speekenbrink, and Hagmayer et al. in this volume, and [6], for more detailed discussions). Much research is needed to ascertain how we learn to manage these cues, the quantitative and qualitative role of cue information in the formation of causal beliefs and in open behavior, and how cues interact when more than one are simultaneously present...


Download PDF

Endorsements



"Open access will revolutionize 21st century knowledge work and accelerate the diffusion of ideas and evidence that support just in time learning and the evolution of thinking in a number of disciplines."


Daniel Pesut
(Indiana University School of Nursing, USA)

"It is important that students and researchers from all over the world can have easy access to relevant, high-standard and timely scientific information. This is exactly what Open Access Journals provide and this is the reason why I support this endeavor."


Jacques Descotes
(Centre Antipoison-Centre de Pharmacovigilance, France)

"Publishing research articles is the key for future scientific progress. Open Access publishing is therefore of utmost importance for wider dissemination of information, and will help serving the best interest of the scientific community."


Patrice Talaga
(UCB S.A., Belgium)

"Open access journals are a novel concept in the medical literature. They offer accessible information to a wide variety of individuals, including physicians, medical students, clinical investigators, and the general public. They are an outstanding source of medical and scientific information."


Jeffrey M. Weinberg
(St. Luke's-Roosevelt Hospital Center, USA)

"Open access journals are extremely useful for graduate students, investigators and all other interested persons to read important scientific articles and subscribe scientific journals. Indeed, the research articles span a wide range of area and of high quality. This is specially a must for researchers belonging to institutions with limited library facility and funding to subscribe scientific journals."


Debomoy K. Lahiri
(Indiana University School of Medicine, USA)

"Open access journals represent a major break-through in publishing. They provide easy access to the latest research on a wide variety of issues. Relevant and timely articles are made available in a fraction of the time taken by more conventional publishers. Articles are of uniformly high quality and written by the world's leading authorities."


Robert Looney
(Naval Postgraduate School, USA)

"Open access journals have transformed the way scientific data is published and disseminated: particularly, whilst ensuring a high quality standard and transparency in the editorial process, they have increased the access to the scientific literature by those researchers that have limited library support or that are working on small budgets."


Richard Reithinger
(Westat, USA)

"Not only do open access journals greatly improve the access to high quality information for scientists in the developing world, it also provides extra exposure for our papers."


J. Ferwerda
(University of Oxford, UK)

"Open Access 'Chemistry' Journals allow the dissemination of knowledge at your finger tips without paying for the scientific content."


Sean L. Kitson
(Almac Sciences, Northern Ireland)

"In principle, all scientific journals should have open access, as should be science itself. Open access journals are very helpful for students, researchers and the general public including people from institutions which do not have library or cannot afford to subscribe scientific journals. The articles are high standard and cover a wide area."


Hubert Wolterbeek
(Delft University of Technology, The Netherlands)

"The widest possible diffusion of information is critical for the advancement of science. In this perspective, open access journals are instrumental in fostering researches and achievements."


Alessandro Laviano
(Sapienza - University of Rome, Italy)

"Open access journals are very useful for all scientists as they can have quick information in the different fields of science."


Philippe Hernigou
(Paris University, France)

"There are many scientists who can not afford the rather expensive subscriptions to scientific journals. Open access journals offer a good alternative for free access to good quality scientific information."


Fidel Toldrá
(Instituto de Agroquimica y Tecnologia de Alimentos, Spain)

"Open access journals have become a fundamental tool for students, researchers, patients and the general public. Many people from institutions which do not have library or cannot afford to subscribe scientific journals benefit of them on a daily basis. The articles are among the best and cover most scientific areas."


M. Bendandi
(University Clinic of Navarre, Spain)

"These journals provide researchers with a platform for rapid, open access scientific communication. The articles are of high quality and broad scope."


Peter Chiba
(University of Vienna, Austria)

"Open access journals are probably one of the most important contributions to promote and diffuse science worldwide."


Jaime Sampaio
(University of Trás-os-Montes e Alto Douro, Portugal)

"Open access journals make up a new and rather revolutionary way to scientific publication. This option opens several quite interesting possibilities to disseminate openly and freely new knowledge and even to facilitate interpersonal communication among scientists."


Eduardo A. Castro
(INIFTA, Argentina)

"Open access journals are freely available online throughout the world, for you to read, download, copy, distribute, and use. The articles published in the open access journals are high quality and cover a wide range of fields."


Kenji Hashimoto
(Chiba University, Japan)

"Open Access journals offer an innovative and efficient way of publication for academics and professionals in a wide range of disciplines. The papers published are of high quality after rigorous peer review and they are Indexed in: major international databases. I read Open Access journals to keep abreast of the recent development in my field of study."


Daniel Shek
(Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong)

"It is a modern trend for publishers to establish open access journals. Researchers, faculty members, and students will be greatly benefited by the new journals of Bentham Science Publishers Ltd. in this category."


Jih Ru Hwu
(National Central University, Taiwan)


Browse Contents



Webmaster Contact: info@benthamopen.net
Copyright © 2019 Bentham Open