The Open Family Studies Journal




(Discontinued)

ISSN: 1874-9224 ― Volume 12, 2020
RESEARCH ARTICLE

Family Size Preferences in a College Student Sample



Brian H. Bornstein*, David K. DiLillo, Hannah Dietrich
1Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska - Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588-0308, United States
2University of Nebraska - Lincoln, Department of Psychology, Lincoln, NE 68588-0308, United States

Abstract

Background:

Family size preferences and birth rate vary across culture, gender, religion, race/ethnicity, and time; yet little is known about how or when people decide how many children to have. Sociobiology suggests that women should invest more time and effort into the decision than men.

Objective:

The study’s purpose is to examine family size preferences in a sample of male and female college students.

Method:

A sample of childless, college-aged participants (n =394; 58.7% women) completed a survey about their desires concerning procreation (e.g., “How many children do you want to have?” “How committed to that number are you?” “How old were you when you picked this number?”).

Results:

Women reported deciding how many children they ideally wanted at a younger age than men, being more committed to that number, and having given it more careful thought. Women also wanted to have their first child at a younger age than men, although men wanted marginally more offspring overall. Participants who used birth control wanted fewer children than those who did not. There were few differences as a function of religion or race/ethnicity.

Conclusion:

Family size preferences were consistent with sociobiological predictions, with women knowing how many children they wanted at a younger age than men, being more committed to a specific number, having given the matter more careful thought, and wanting to start childbearing at a younger age. Thus, despite recent cultural and societal changes, biological imperatives still appear to influence decision making about this most fundamental of behaviors.

Keywords: Family size, Procreation, Gender differences, Sociobiology.


Article Information


Identifiers and Pagination:

Year: 2017
Volume: 09
First Page: 31
Last Page: 36
Publisher Id: TOFAMSJ-9-31
DOI: 10.2174/1874922401709010031

Article History:

Received Date: 10/01/2017
Revision Received Date: 25/04/2017
Acceptance Date: 28/04/2017
Electronic publication date: 31/07/2017
Collection year: 2017

© 2017 Bornstein et al.

open-access license: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Public License (CC-BY 4.0), a copy of which is available at: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode. This license permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.


* Address correspondence to this author at the 335 Burnett Hall, Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska - Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588-0308, United States; Tel: 402-472-3721; Fax: 402-472-4637; E-mail: bbornstein2@unl.edu





1. INTRODUCTION

Family size is largely a matter of choice, at least in Western countries. There are various indicators of family size, such as birth rate (births/1000 population/year) and fertility rate (children born/woman). Both measures vary widely across as well as within societies [1Kotkin J. The next hundred million. New York: Penguin Press 2010.]. For example, the fertility rate (as of 2014) is over 5 in many African countries; approximately 2 in the U.S.; less than 2 in Australia, Canada, Russia, and most of Europe; and close to 1 in many Asian countries [2CIA World Factbook. Country comparison: Total fertility rate. Available from: http://www.indexmundi.com/g/r.aspx?v=31 2014. Jan 1 [retrieved 2016 Dec 21]]. Use of birth control measures roughly parallels family size; countries (or racial/ethnic groups within countries) that have higher birth control utilization have correspondingly lower birth rates [3Balbo N, Billari FC, Mills M. Fertility in advanced societies: A review of research. Eur J Popul/Rev Eur de Démographie 2013; 29(1): 1-38.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10680-012-9277-y]
].

Within a society, there are significant demographic differences. In the U.S., Whites have a lower birth rate than Blacks, Hispanics, and Asians [1Kotkin J. The next hundred million. New York: Penguin Press 2010., 4Martin JA, Hamilton BE, Osterman MJ, Curtin SC, Matthews TJ. Births: final data for 2013. Natl Vital Stat Rep 2015; 64(1): 1-65.
[PMID: 25603115]
]. With respect to religion, there is a widespread perception that Catholics tend to have large families, and to some extent, Catholics are more likely to view a larger family as ideal [5Nobbe CE, Okraku IO. Male-female differences in family size preferences among college students. Soc Biol 1974; 21(3): 279-89.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19485565.1974.9988122] [PMID: 4450120]
-7Westoff CF, Potvin RH. College women and fertility values. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press 1967.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/9781400876051]
]; yet religious differences in fertility are much less than in the past and are possibly now non-existent altogether, at least in the U.S [8Frejka T, Westoff CF. Religion, religiousness and fertility in the U.S. and in Europe. Paper presented at the 2006 annual meeting of the PAA (Los Angeles, CA 2006). Available from: http://www.d.umn.edu/~okuhlke/Fall%202006%20Classes/GEOG%203762%20Europe/ Readings/Week%2013%20-%20Demography/ EUdemogr1.pdf. 2006.].

Men and women likely make decisions about procreation quite differently, due to sex roles, family influences, and biology. For example, women’s fertility and family size preferences are related to their sex-role attitudes [9Granberg D. Family size preferences and sexual permissiveness as factors differentiating abortion activists. Soc Psychol Q 1982; 45: 15-23.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3033670]
-11Wrigley AP, Stokes CS. Sex-role ideology, selected life plans, and family size preferences: Further evidence. J Comp Fam Stud 1977; 8: 391-400.]. A 2009 study found that females who intended to have children were significantly more likely to identify having a “biological” drive to have children compared to males who intended to have children and both males and females who did not intend to have children [12Landridge D, Sheeran P, Connolly K. Understanding the reasons for parenthood. J Reprod Infant Psychol 2005; 23(2): 121-33.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02646830500129438]
]. Consistent with this finding, a core tenet of sociobiological theory is that women should be more selective in mating than men, by virtue of their greater investment in terms of material and physiological resources [13Wilson EO. Sociobiology: The new synthesis (25th anniversary edition). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press 2000.]. There is evidence for this differential selectivity across the animal kingdom, including in humans [13Wilson EO. Sociobiology: The new synthesis (25th anniversary edition). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press 2000.].

Despite the clear relevance of family size to well-being and resource utilization, at both the micro (e.g., individual, clan) and macro (e.g., village, society) levels, surprisingly little research has addressed exactly how and when individuals decide how large a family to have. What research exists has focused primarily on married persons [6West KK, Morgan LA. Public perceptions of the ideal number of children for contemporary families. Popul Environ 1987; 9: 160-72.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01259306]
, 14Beckman LJ. Fertility preferences and social exchange theory. J Appl Soc Psychol 1979; 9: 147-69.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1979.tb00801.x]
-16Miller WB, Pasta DJ. Motivational and nonmotivational determinants of child-number desires. Popul Environ 1993; 15: 113-38.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02209405]
]; however, individuals appear to enter marriage with some expectations about childbearing and family size already formed [17Sabatello EF. Premarital experiences and family size expectations of U.S. Jewish women: Some exploratory comparisons. J Popul Stud 1985; 19: 149-56.]. Studies of older, married individuals are limited, moreover, by the fact that the strongest predictor of the number of children desired is the number of children one already has [16Miller WB, Pasta DJ. Motivational and nonmotivational determinants of child-number desires. Popul Environ 1993; 15: 113-38.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02209405]
, 18Gwako E. Married women’s ideal family size preferences and family planning practices: Evidence from rural Kenya. Soc Sci J 1997; 34: 369-82.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0362-3319(97)90035-5]
].

The scant research that has addressed the preferences of younger, single individuals has focused primarily on women [11Wrigley AP, Stokes CS. Sex-role ideology, selected life plans, and family size preferences: Further evidence. J Comp Fam Stud 1977; 8: 391-400.]. Some research has compared the family size preferences of young, unmarried men and women [5Nobbe CE, Okraku IO. Male-female differences in family size preferences among college students. Soc Biol 1974; 21(3): 279-89.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19485565.1974.9988122] [PMID: 4450120]
]. This research found that single women desired larger families than single men, a difference that it attributed to sex roles; however, most of the literature in the area is dated, and sex roles have changed considerably since the 1960s and early 1970s. For example, in 1970, 40% of women in the U.S. were employed in the labor force, compared to 56% in 2008 [19U.S. Department of Labor. Women in the labor force: A databook. Washington, DC: Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2009. Available from: http://www.bls.gov/cps/ wlf-databook-2009.pdf 2009.]. In addition, prior research has not addressed the age and processes by which family size preferences are formed. The present study addresses these gaps in the literature by examining family size preferences in a sample of childless male and female college students.

2. HYPOTHESIS

Based on the differential resource investment between men and women, we formulated three hypotheses:

H1. Women would form a family size preference at an earlier age than men.

H2. Women would be more committed than men to having a specific number of children.

H3. Women would anticipate experiencing greater regret if they did not have their desired number.

With respect to the number of desired children, there are competing hypotheses. On the one hand, socialization norms would lead women to want more children [5Nobbe CE, Okraku IO. Male-female differences in family size preferences among college students. Soc Biol 1974; 21(3): 279-89.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19485565.1974.9988122] [PMID: 4450120]
]; on the other hand, differential resource investment would lead men to want more children. Thus, we make no hypothesis regarding a sex difference for number of desired children. We also explored family size preferences as a function of race/ethnicity, religion, and birth control use.

3. METHOD

3.1. Participants

Participants were 394 childless undergraduates (58.7% women, 85.8% White, 93.4% unmarried), recruited from psychology courses (7 participants with children were excluded from the analyses). Males and females did not differ in terms of the number of children they grew up with or marital status, though males were slightly older (Ms = 20.41 vs. 19.50 years; t[352] = 3.99, p < .05, η2 = .04). (In this and subsequent comparisons, M refers to the group mean; t refers to the independent t-test statistic used to test for differences between groups; and η2 is a measure of effect size analogous to R2.)

3.2. Materials

Eleven questions assessed participants’ desires and beliefs about having children (using 7-pt scales unless otherwise specified): 1) How many children do you want to have? (allowing a response of any number, including 0; participants could also respond “unsure”); 2) How committed to that number are you? 3) Why did you choose this specific number? (free response); 4) How old were you when you picked this number? (hereafter referred to as age of decision); 5) Was the decision something you thought through carefully or a gut feeling? 6) How many girls and boys? 7) How important in choosing a mate/spouse was/would be that person’s preferences? 8) How much regret would you experience if you ended up having a) more or b) fewer children? 9) If you desire multiple children, what is the optimal age difference? (free response); 10) At what age would you like to have your a) first and b) last child? 11) How much do financial considerations affect your decision?

Participants who were married or in a long-term committed relationship answered four additional questions (e.g., “How much did you talk about this issue with your partner before you were married, versus after?”). Participants also provided basic demographic information and reported whether they had been sexually active in the last six months; and, if so, which form of birth control they usually used.

3.3. Procedure

Participants gave informed consent, then completed the questionnaire in small groups, with each participant working individually. The questionnaire took 15-20 minutes to complete. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board for adherence to ethical principles in the use of human participants.

4. RESULTS

The data were analyzed using SPSS; degrees of freedom vary because participants had the option not to answer individual questions. All but seven participants indicated a desire for children, with a majority indicating a preference for two (33.9%) or three (35.2%) children (M = 3.08, SD = 1.59). Similarly, the average number of children in home of upbringing was around three (M = 3.02, SD = 1.28).

Table 1 shows the major findings. There was a marginally significant difference in the number of children desired by men (M = 3.28) and women (M = 2.94), t(267) = 1.92, p = .06, η2 = .01. As hypothesized, women (M = 15.82) reported making a decision earlier in life about the number of children they wanted to have compared to their male counterparts (M = 17.55), t(348) = 5.10, p < .01, η2 = .07. Women showed a higher degree of commitment (M = 4.92) than men (M = 4.56) to their desired number, t(346) = 2.37, p = .02, η2 = .01.

Table 1
Desired number of children, age of decision, and degree of commitment as a function of demographic and behavioral characteristics.


Men and women differed significantly in their response to the question, “Was the decision something you thought through carefully or a gut feeling?” t(360) = 2.05, p < .05, η2 = .01, with women more likely to have given the decision careful thought (Ms = 4.04 [SD = 1.45] vs. 3.72 [SD = 1.52]). Women wanted their first child at a significantly younger age (M = 26.05, SD = 2.11) than men (M = 26.61, SD = 2.78), t(352) = 2.18, p < .05, η2 = .01, but there was no significant difference between women and men regarding the age at which they wanted to have their last child, t(341) = 1.91.

When asked how much regret they would experience if they had either more or fewer children than desired, men and women did not differ significantly, ts < 1.61. Participants predicted they would experience less regret over having more children (M = 2.39, SD = 1.50) compared to having fewer children than desired (M = 3.21, SD = 1.86), t(389) = 7.27, p < .01, η2 = .12.

As an overwhelming majority of the participants identified as White (85.8%), ethnicity was coded as being either White or racial/ethnic minority. There was no significant difference in the number of children desired by Whites and minorities, t(393) = .42. Race/ethnicity also was unrelated to the age at which participants reported deciding on the number of children they wanted to have, t(377) = 1.17, or their degree of commitment, t(374) = .86.

Although data were collected for a variety of religious groups, participants predominantly identified as being Catholic (31.2%) or Protestant (56.4%). There was no significant difference in the number of children desired by Catholics and Protestants, t(349) = 1.04. Catholics and Protestants also did not differ on the age at which they reported deciding on the number of children they wanted to have, t(335) = 1.44. Catholics and Protestants showed a small (η2 = .02) but significant difference in how committed they were to the number of children they wanted to have, with Catholics less committed (M = 4.55) than Protestants (M = 4.96), t(334) = 2.55, p = .01.

Over half of the participants (57.9%) indicated that they had been sexually active in the past 6 months. Of these, 95% were using some form of birth control (BC). One’s decision to use BC may reflect childbearing intentions, but because of the small number of non-BC users, these analyses are exploratory. There was a small (η2 = .03) but significant difference in the number of children desired by BC users (M = 2.94) and non-BC users (M = 4.10), t(230) = 2.45, p = .02. BC users and nonusers did not differ on the age at which they reported deciding on the number of children they wanted to have, t(222) = 1.26, or their level of commitment, t(222) = .00.

5. DISCUSSION

The vast majority of participants desired children, which is consistent with both biological imperatives and gender roles for both men and women [13Wilson EO. Sociobiology: The new synthesis (25th anniversary edition). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press 2000.]. The number desired by most participants—two or three—is a robust finding in recent American samples [6West KK, Morgan LA. Public perceptions of the ideal number of children for contemporary families. Popul Environ 1987; 9: 160-72.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01259306]
, 11Wrigley AP, Stokes CS. Sex-role ideology, selected life plans, and family size preferences: Further evidence. J Comp Fam Stud 1977; 8: 391-400., 20Hagewen KJ, Morgan SP. Intended and ideal family size in the United States, 1970–2002. Popul Dev Rev 2005; 31(3): 507-27.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1728-4457.2005.00081.x] [PMID: 20376334]
] and consistent with the current U.S. fertility rate [1Kotkin J. The next hundred million. New York: Penguin Press 2010., 2CIA World Factbook. Country comparison: Total fertility rate. Available from: http://www.indexmundi.com/g/r.aspx?v=31 2014. Jan 1 [retrieved 2016 Dec 21]]. Even though most participants were in their late teens or early twenties, nearly all had a specific number of children in mind, and they tended to be strongly committed to that number (the mean score on the commitment question was above the midpoint for both men and women). Nonetheless, there were some significant differences in terms of demographic characteristics, especially as a function of participants’ gender.

Women reported deciding how many children they wanted at a younger age than men, by nearly two years. Women also reported being more firmly committed to a specific number, desiring slightly fewer children than men on average, and wanting to have their first child at a younger age. The gender difference in number of desired children contradicts a previous research finding that women preferred slightly more children than men [5Nobbe CE, Okraku IO. Male-female differences in family size preferences among college students. Soc Biol 1974; 21(3): 279-89.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19485565.1974.9988122] [PMID: 4450120]
]. A likely explanation of this discrepancy is societal changes over time. The research reviewed by Nobbe and Okraku [5Nobbe CE, Okraku IO. Male-female differences in family size preferences among college students. Soc Biol 1974; 21(3): 279-89.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19485565.1974.9988122] [PMID: 4450120]
] was conducted in the 1960s and early 1970s, at the beginning of the women’s liberation movement in the U.S. Since that time, many more women have entered the workforce, as traditional sex roles have relaxed; and working women, as well as those with less traditional sex-role attitudes, desire fewer children [14Beckman LJ. Fertility preferences and social exchange theory. J Appl Soc Psychol 1979; 9: 147-69.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1979.tb00801.x]
, 15Beckman LJ. The relationship between sex roles, fertility, and family size preferences. Psychol Women Q 1979; 4(1): 43-6.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1979.tb00698.x] [PMID: 12336874]
, 21Kaufman G. Do gender role attitudes matter? Family formation and dissolution among traditional and egalitarian men and women. J Fam Issues 2000; 21(1): 128-44.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/019251300021001006]
]. These trends likely contribute to the closing, and even reversal, of the male-female gap in family size preferences.

Both men and women anticipated experiencing more regret at having too few than too many children. This tendency could reflect a framing effect, whereby “too few” involves imagining taking away something one already has, which is perceived as a loss [22Kahneman D, Tversky A. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 1979; 47: 263-91.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1914185]
]. Of course, participants did not already have children, but in their imagined subjective future state, it would nonetheless be perceived as a loss, compared to the incremental “gain” of having more children than desired [23Beach LR. Image theory: Theoretical and empirical foundations. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates 1998.].

Participants who used birth control wanted fewer children than participants who did not. This finding is consistent with other research showing that the availability of family planning measures is associated with intentions to have fewer children [18Gwako E. Married women’s ideal family size preferences and family planning practices: Evidence from rural Kenya. Soc Sci J 1997; 34: 369-82.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0362-3319(97)90035-5]
, 24Bongaarts J. Fertility and reproductive preferences in post-transitional societies. Popul Dev Rev 2001; 27: 260-81.]. A variety of factors go into the decision whether to use birth control [25Philliber S, Namerow PB, Kaye JW, Kunkes CH. Pregnancy risk taking among adolescents. J Adolesc Res 1986; 1: 463-81.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/074355488614011]
]. Contraceptive use and family size preferences are likely part of a constellation of variables including sex-role attitudes, career aspirations, and other factors [12Landridge D, Sheeran P, Connolly K. Understanding the reasons for parenthood. J Reprod Infant Psychol 2005; 23(2): 121-33.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02646830500129438]
, 14Beckman LJ. Fertility preferences and social exchange theory. J Appl Soc Psychol 1979; 9: 147-69.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1979.tb00801.x]
, 15Beckman LJ. The relationship between sex roles, fertility, and family size preferences. Psychol Women Q 1979; 4(1): 43-6.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1979.tb00698.x] [PMID: 12336874]
]. Contraceptives allow women (and their partners) to be more selective about when they want offspring and how many of them to have [6West KK, Morgan LA. Public perceptions of the ideal number of children for contemporary families. Popul Environ 1987; 9: 160-72.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01259306]
].

5.1. Limitations and Future Directions

The present study has several limitations. First, the participants were undergraduate students and therefore reasonably well-educated and mostly middle-class; the sample also did not have a great deal of racial, religious, or geographical diversity. Although there might be broad differences with a more diverse sample-such as less educated, lower-SES individuals desiring more children [15Beckman LJ. The relationship between sex roles, fertility, and family size preferences. Psychol Women Q 1979; 4(1): 43-6.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1979.tb00698.x] [PMID: 12336874]
], or variations across racial/ethnic groups [1Kotkin J. The next hundred million. New York: Penguin Press 2010., 26Unger JB, Molina GB. Desired family size and son preference among Hispanic women of low socioeconomic status. Fam Plann Perspect 1997; 29(6): 284-7.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2953419] [PMID: 9429876]
] - we expect that the pattern of findings, especially regarding gender differences, would remain the same.

The present sample also consisted entirely of Americans. Fertility and family size preferences differ, often quite dramatically, across societies [18Gwako E. Married women’s ideal family size preferences and family planning practices: Evidence from rural Kenya. Soc Sci J 1997; 34: 369-82.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0362-3319(97)90035-5]
, 27Doherty JP, Norton EC, Veney JE. China’s one-child policy: the economic choices and consequences faced by pregnant women. Soc Sci Med 2001; 52(5): 745-61.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00175-1] [PMID: 11218178]
]. Nations with social policies different from the U.S., such as welfare states, might inculcate in their citizens a preference for more or fewer children, for a variety of reasons. Some societal changes relevant to family planning, such as industrialization, availability of birth control measures, and government policies, occur relatively quickly (i.e., over a few generations). Sociobiological factors, on the other hand-such as an evolutionary basis for sex differences in family planning and family size preferences-are more stable, owing to their long evolutionary history. Thus, we would expect a comparable pattern of findings from other countries, especially those with more traditional sex roles.

Memory bias is another limiting factor. In general, “memory for when” is not highly accurate [28Neath I, Surprenant AM. Human memory. 2nd ed. Belmont, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth 2003., 29Thompson CP, Skowronski JJ, Lee DJ. Telescoping in dating naturally occurring events. Mem Cognit 1988; 16(5): 461-8.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03214227] [PMID: 3173095]
], and that is likely to be complicated further when asking participants to pinpoint the timing of a decision-such as how many children they want to have-that is probably made gradually and not at a discrete point in time. This problem is offset somewhat by the fact that the mean age of decision was not too long ago for our college-aged sample.

CONCLUSION

Overall, college students’ family size preferences were consistent with sociobiological predictions. Women reported deciding how many children they wanted at a younger age than men, being more committed to a specific number, having given the matter more careful thought, and wanting to start childbearing at a younger age. Thus, despite recent cultural and societal changes, biological imperatives still appear to influence decision making about this most fundamental of behaviors.

ETHICS APPROVAL AND CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE

Not applicable.

HUMAN AND ANIMAL RIGHTS

No Animals/Humans were used for studies that are the basis of this research.

CONSENT FOR PUBLICATION

Not applicable.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors confirm that this article content has no conflict of interest.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We are grateful to Susan Bornstein for helpful research suggestions, and to Kelly Clevenger and Erin Zwart for assistance with data collection.

REFERENCES

[1] Kotkin J. The next hundred million. New York: Penguin Press 2010.
[2] CIA World Factbook. Country comparison: Total fertility rate. Available from: http://www.indexmundi.com/g/r.aspx?v=31 2014. Jan 1 [retrieved 2016 Dec 21]
[3] Balbo N, Billari FC, Mills M. Fertility in advanced societies: A review of research. Eur J Popul/Rev Eur de Démographie 2013; 29(1): 1-38.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10680-012-9277-y]
[4] Martin JA, Hamilton BE, Osterman MJ, Curtin SC, Matthews TJ. Births: final data for 2013. Natl Vital Stat Rep 2015; 64(1): 1-65.
[PMID: 25603115]
[5] Nobbe CE, Okraku IO. Male-female differences in family size preferences among college students. Soc Biol 1974; 21(3): 279-89.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19485565.1974.9988122] [PMID: 4450120]
[6] West KK, Morgan LA. Public perceptions of the ideal number of children for contemporary families. Popul Environ 1987; 9: 160-72.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01259306]
[7] Westoff CF, Potvin RH. College women and fertility values. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press 1967.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/9781400876051]
[8] Frejka T, Westoff CF. Religion, religiousness and fertility in the U.S. and in Europe. Paper presented at the 2006 annual meeting of the PAA (Los Angeles, CA 2006). Available from: http://www.d.umn.edu/~okuhlke/Fall%202006%20Classes/GEOG%203762%20Europe/ Readings/Week%2013%20-%20Demography/ EUdemogr1.pdf. 2006.
[9] Granberg D. Family size preferences and sexual permissiveness as factors differentiating abortion activists. Soc Psychol Q 1982; 45: 15-23.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3033670]
[10] Thornton A, Camburn D. Fertility, sex role attitudes, and labor force participation. Psychol Women Q 1979; 4(1): 61-80.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1979.tb00699.x] [PMID: 12336875]
[11] Wrigley AP, Stokes CS. Sex-role ideology, selected life plans, and family size preferences: Further evidence. J Comp Fam Stud 1977; 8: 391-400.
[12] Landridge D, Sheeran P, Connolly K. Understanding the reasons for parenthood. J Reprod Infant Psychol 2005; 23(2): 121-33.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02646830500129438]
[13] Wilson EO. Sociobiology: The new synthesis (25th anniversary edition). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press 2000.
[14] Beckman LJ. Fertility preferences and social exchange theory. J Appl Soc Psychol 1979; 9: 147-69.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1979.tb00801.x]
[15] Beckman LJ. The relationship between sex roles, fertility, and family size preferences. Psychol Women Q 1979; 4(1): 43-6.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1979.tb00698.x] [PMID: 12336874]
[16] Miller WB, Pasta DJ. Motivational and nonmotivational determinants of child-number desires. Popul Environ 1993; 15: 113-38.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02209405]
[17] Sabatello EF. Premarital experiences and family size expectations of U.S. Jewish women: Some exploratory comparisons. J Popul Stud 1985; 19: 149-56.
[18] Gwako E. Married women’s ideal family size preferences and family planning practices: Evidence from rural Kenya. Soc Sci J 1997; 34: 369-82.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0362-3319(97)90035-5]
[19] U.S. Department of Labor. Women in the labor force: A databook. Washington, DC: Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2009. Available from: http://www.bls.gov/cps/ wlf-databook-2009.pdf 2009.
[20] Hagewen KJ, Morgan SP. Intended and ideal family size in the United States, 1970–2002. Popul Dev Rev 2005; 31(3): 507-27.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1728-4457.2005.00081.x] [PMID: 20376334]
[21] Kaufman G. Do gender role attitudes matter? Family formation and dissolution among traditional and egalitarian men and women. J Fam Issues 2000; 21(1): 128-44.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/019251300021001006]
[22] Kahneman D, Tversky A. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 1979; 47: 263-91.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1914185]
[23] Beach LR. Image theory: Theoretical and empirical foundations. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates 1998.
[24] Bongaarts J. Fertility and reproductive preferences in post-transitional societies. Popul Dev Rev 2001; 27: 260-81.
[25] Philliber S, Namerow PB, Kaye JW, Kunkes CH. Pregnancy risk taking among adolescents. J Adolesc Res 1986; 1: 463-81.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/074355488614011]
[26] Unger JB, Molina GB. Desired family size and son preference among Hispanic women of low socioeconomic status. Fam Plann Perspect 1997; 29(6): 284-7.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2953419] [PMID: 9429876]
[27] Doherty JP, Norton EC, Veney JE. China’s one-child policy: the economic choices and consequences faced by pregnant women. Soc Sci Med 2001; 52(5): 745-61.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00175-1] [PMID: 11218178]
[28] Neath I, Surprenant AM. Human memory. 2nd ed. Belmont, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth 2003.
[29] Thompson CP, Skowronski JJ, Lee DJ. Telescoping in dating naturally occurring events. Mem Cognit 1988; 16(5): 461-8.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03214227] [PMID: 3173095]
Track Your Manuscript:


Endorsements



"Open access will revolutionize 21st century knowledge work and accelerate the diffusion of ideas and evidence that support just in time learning and the evolution of thinking in a number of disciplines."


Daniel Pesut
(Indiana University School of Nursing, USA)

"It is important that students and researchers from all over the world can have easy access to relevant, high-standard and timely scientific information. This is exactly what Open Access Journals provide and this is the reason why I support this endeavor."


Jacques Descotes
(Centre Antipoison-Centre de Pharmacovigilance, France)

"Publishing research articles is the key for future scientific progress. Open Access publishing is therefore of utmost importance for wider dissemination of information, and will help serving the best interest of the scientific community."


Patrice Talaga
(UCB S.A., Belgium)

"Open access journals are a novel concept in the medical literature. They offer accessible information to a wide variety of individuals, including physicians, medical students, clinical investigators, and the general public. They are an outstanding source of medical and scientific information."


Jeffrey M. Weinberg
(St. Luke's-Roosevelt Hospital Center, USA)

"Open access journals are extremely useful for graduate students, investigators and all other interested persons to read important scientific articles and subscribe scientific journals. Indeed, the research articles span a wide range of area and of high quality. This is specially a must for researchers belonging to institutions with limited library facility and funding to subscribe scientific journals."


Debomoy K. Lahiri
(Indiana University School of Medicine, USA)

"Open access journals represent a major break-through in publishing. They provide easy access to the latest research on a wide variety of issues. Relevant and timely articles are made available in a fraction of the time taken by more conventional publishers. Articles are of uniformly high quality and written by the world's leading authorities."


Robert Looney
(Naval Postgraduate School, USA)

"Open access journals have transformed the way scientific data is published and disseminated: particularly, whilst ensuring a high quality standard and transparency in the editorial process, they have increased the access to the scientific literature by those researchers that have limited library support or that are working on small budgets."


Richard Reithinger
(Westat, USA)

"Not only do open access journals greatly improve the access to high quality information for scientists in the developing world, it also provides extra exposure for our papers."


J. Ferwerda
(University of Oxford, UK)

"Open Access 'Chemistry' Journals allow the dissemination of knowledge at your finger tips without paying for the scientific content."


Sean L. Kitson
(Almac Sciences, Northern Ireland)

"In principle, all scientific journals should have open access, as should be science itself. Open access journals are very helpful for students, researchers and the general public including people from institutions which do not have library or cannot afford to subscribe scientific journals. The articles are high standard and cover a wide area."


Hubert Wolterbeek
(Delft University of Technology, The Netherlands)

"The widest possible diffusion of information is critical for the advancement of science. In this perspective, open access journals are instrumental in fostering researches and achievements."


Alessandro Laviano
(Sapienza - University of Rome, Italy)

"Open access journals are very useful for all scientists as they can have quick information in the different fields of science."


Philippe Hernigou
(Paris University, France)

"There are many scientists who can not afford the rather expensive subscriptions to scientific journals. Open access journals offer a good alternative for free access to good quality scientific information."


Fidel Toldrá
(Instituto de Agroquimica y Tecnologia de Alimentos, Spain)

"Open access journals have become a fundamental tool for students, researchers, patients and the general public. Many people from institutions which do not have library or cannot afford to subscribe scientific journals benefit of them on a daily basis. The articles are among the best and cover most scientific areas."


M. Bendandi
(University Clinic of Navarre, Spain)

"These journals provide researchers with a platform for rapid, open access scientific communication. The articles are of high quality and broad scope."


Peter Chiba
(University of Vienna, Austria)

"Open access journals are probably one of the most important contributions to promote and diffuse science worldwide."


Jaime Sampaio
(University of Trás-os-Montes e Alto Douro, Portugal)

"Open access journals make up a new and rather revolutionary way to scientific publication. This option opens several quite interesting possibilities to disseminate openly and freely new knowledge and even to facilitate interpersonal communication among scientists."


Eduardo A. Castro
(INIFTA, Argentina)

"Open access journals are freely available online throughout the world, for you to read, download, copy, distribute, and use. The articles published in the open access journals are high quality and cover a wide range of fields."


Kenji Hashimoto
(Chiba University, Japan)

"Open Access journals offer an innovative and efficient way of publication for academics and professionals in a wide range of disciplines. The papers published are of high quality after rigorous peer review and they are Indexed in: major international databases. I read Open Access journals to keep abreast of the recent development in my field of study."


Daniel Shek
(Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong)

"It is a modern trend for publishers to establish open access journals. Researchers, faculty members, and students will be greatly benefited by the new journals of Bentham Science Publishers Ltd. in this category."


Jih Ru Hwu
(National Central University, Taiwan)


Browse Contents




Webmaster Contact: info@benthamopen.net
Copyright © 2023 Bentham Open